Schoener's paper may be the first proper review paper that we've come across. It's dense, long and spattered with math, and one gets the feeling while reading that Schoener has spent a long time pouring over his colleagues' work. The paper itself spans a number of topics related to feeding strategy, and in the interest of brevity I will try to be short in summation.
General Model of Feeding Strategy
Schoener starts with the basics of time vs energy, describing two types of feeders. Type I is an organism that searches for food while partaking in other activities such as searching for mates, predator avoidance or displacing invaders, and therefore does not expend energy exclusively on searching. Type II is an organism for which searching for food and other activities are mutually exclusive. Both types must optimize their diet based on the following equation for individual prey items:
(e/t)=(Potential Energy - pursuit cost - handling costs)/(Pursuit time + handling time)
If any one prey item's net energy is greater than the feeder's energy requirement (M), then the feeder should focus on that item. If a single prey item does not fulfill the feeder's energy requirement, the feeder should add different items in order of highest to lowest (e/t) to reach its energy requirements. For Type II feeders, any time spent traveling to feeding grounds before any energy is gained must also be taken into account. For them, a balance must be found between searching for food and performing other activities. Either type should optimize their diet in order to increase fitness. We see the gain in fitness by looking at the relation of increased energy to basal metabolic rate. Any diet must satisfy basal metabolic rate, which is related to body weight (W), before the organism can focus on growing or breeding. Large animals tend to be more efficient in storing fat reserves, and so can generally focus less on this basic upkeep. Once BMR is satisfied, optimal diet will benefit organisms by increasing growth rate, and potentially decreasing the time before reproduction, in which case their optimized diet will also benefit their offspring, increase broods per season and individuals per brood. Generally speaking, an organism should try to maximize energy obtained from their diet, however, the benefit from maximizing energy must be balanced with the benefit from performing other tasks such as mating. After all, it doesn't matter how much energy you could potentially give to your offspring if you never actually have any. As such, Schoener points out that feeding time and success in mating are inversely proportional. This is not to say that optimal diet is an unwavering standard for species. Each organism, regardless of feeding type, moves through a progression of feeding periods in which the organism should strive to optimize their net energy based on the requirements put upon them in that feeding period. The optimal diet should be recalculated every period. Optimization can be further viewed based on whether the organism is a time minimizer (generally organisms with a fixed reproductive output) or an energy maximizer.
Optimal Diet
Schoener here moves on to an examination of the optimal diet. In order to be feeding optimally, organisms must choose an optimal diet, which is based on search time, pursuit time, search and pursuit energy expenditure, pursuit and capture success, potential caloric content and relative abundance of food items. Schoener presents a number of different models for optimal diet proposed by other authors. Hollings examines which prey would be mechanically easiest to predate, MacArthur and Pianka look at the forces of natural selection on eating as a function of pursuit time and search time. Emlen considered when an organism should pass on a prey item if they were aware of another prey item available and Levins & MacArthur examine how much selectivity in diet is most beneficial to the production of offspring. The general consensus being that food scarcity should increase the variety of prey items eaten. Feeders should take larger items closer to their origin before moving further to eat smaller items, and smaller predators should take smaller prey. Furthermore, the more you travel to find food, the more specialized you should be to increase your energy intake; larger organisms should be more specialist for the same reason. Generalism is favored, however, where food items are affected deferentially and unpredictably. Interestingly, Schoener asserts that when all food items are affected equally by feeders, feeders should converge in size.
Schoener concludes the section with a short discussion of feeding rates, siting Holling's "functional response", which maps three separate intake rates based on food density. The first, linear model based on Lotka-Volterra, being most similar to a filter feeder, is an organism for which eating does not interfere with searching for food. The second is an organism which is limited by its ability to process food. We might think of something like a lion, which could theoretically catch many items when prey density is high, but then must stop capturing prey in order to eat. Finally, the third, making a similar plot to the second, is an organism that is limited by food processing, but also either learns or switches food based on density. This would be something like a honey bee, which must first search for food, but then once food is found remembers its location.
Optimal Forage Space
Here we explore spacial range for an organism, which is again based on food density, as well as selectivity and metabolism of the feeder. Schoener explains that for any area in which food items are not uniformly distributed, the range size must exceed the size expected based on the above criteria to adjust for food patchiness. Here an organism's connectedness comes into play; the range size for a more connected organism being smaller than that of a less connected organism. Larger organisms should have larger ranges. Carnivores generally have larger ranges than hunting herbivores, which have larger ranges than browsers and grazers, which is based on level of specialization and food density. Efficient pursuers should have larger ranges because it is less energetically expensive for them to pursue prey. Rather than increasing range size, a decrease in food density causes a decrease in the range of items taken from the same area- organisms become more specialist. However, according to "compression hypothesis", the range of patches searched by feeders will decrease when food items are reduced deferentially based on the quality of the patch. Finally Schoener discusses the relation of food resources on territoriality, and the energy an organism should expend to protect a food source. Basically, when invasion is very low, and food resources are dense, there is no reason to protect food. Likewise, when invasion is common and food is scarce, the benefit of protecting a food source is outweighed by the energy expended chasing off invaders. Therefore, range should only be protected when invader frequency is below a certain level, and when food is neither in severe abundance or rarity.
Optimal Feeding Period
Schoener very briefly discusses when organisms should expand their feeding period, or should utilize more energetically expensive feeding strategies. Generally, when food is scarce, when the feeder has a large energy requirement, or when the feeder has the best opportunity to reproduce, they should expand the feeding period.
Optimal Foraging Group Size
Finally Schoener explores the role of gregarious behavior in feeding strategy. He presents three potential reasons why organisms may forage in groups and support behind the theories. Each depends on foraging efficiency, risk of predation and the defensible area and its unit cost of defense. The first type of group foraging decreases foraging efficiency, but it is energetically beneficial because it improves another aspect of the organism's life, such as navigation, breeding potential and a decreased risk of predation. The second type has no affect on foraging efficiency, but occurs where food resources are clumped such that multiple individuals can obtain more food together than any could foraging elsewhere alone. And the third type of group foraging increases foraging efficiency by flushing or driving prey, or simply by increasing the size of the food item that can be taken by the feeders collectively. All types of group foraging can be further beneficial by decreasing the overlap in home ranges, thereby decreasing the energy and time spent protecting the area, and decreasing time spent searching a large area. Groups can collectively increase the maximum range of any individual.
Questions:
Schoener makes the assumption in the general model that any organism that does not feed during a period has a reproductive output of 0. Obviously if an organism starves to death, or has no energy to mate, this is true. Are there cases where it is not?
Is the expenditure of energy in hunting really proportional to BMR? Are there any organisms that function outside the proposed (lambda) of 0.5 to 1?
Schoener asserts that larger organisms should be more specialized and have larger ranges. Does our knowledge of the past confirm this?
Why would feeders converge in size when food items are uniformly reduced?
I am really interested in the group foraging behavior advantages described. This seems to give a big selective advantage to individuals that participate (though food is not necessarily split equally). The greater range, greater search efficiency, better defense of food, and lower competition with other groups makes this a huge advantage. On another note I am wondering how this feeding strategy work might relate to microbes. This is a very predator centric paper, yet with ectomycorrhizae for example I see certain parallels: individuals occupy “territory” on a root keeping other mycorrhizae from interacting with the plant at that point. The plant essentially acts as a food source for the fungus. A propagule or hyphae also has to be in contact with root tissue to really establish somewhat akin to the search area. Timing matters too as certain conditions and/or temperature ranges are ideal for some microbes while others are excluded. I see lots of parallels to a system that is by no means a predator prey relationship, but rather a true mutualism in most cases.
ReplyDeleteThis paper has many ideas to entertain that are very interesting to me. I agree with Eric that the ideas about group foraging behavior is intriguing - while all seem beneficial, is there one strategy that really benefits the forager the most? Also, this article brought me back to what we talked about in class on Tuesday about which trophic level has the most available energy. While predators are hunting for prey, does their output of energy spent searching for food outweigh the energy they will get back? And while doing so, is there an overall net gain or loss of energy that accumulates over time? This idea seems like it could be applied to any trophic level, and if it doesn't I would be interested in talking about it more. Also, do mutualisms result in a net gain or loss for either species involved? Taking into account all of the energy associated with both species finding each other, etc.
ReplyDeleteI agree with you Ali, energy gained should exceed energy spent at any trophic level. The only example given kind of fits net loss might be seasonal variation. Although not an example of true net loss, an example would include organisms that use fat storages for energy during winter hibernation (net loss). Good question!
DeleteIt's interesting to think about mutualistic relationships and if there is a net gain or loss for each species. My thoughts are that there is a net gain for both, otherwise there would not be this relationship in the first place. But now there is more evidence pointing to a lot of organisms being "cheats" in this relationship, and one species can sometimes take more than it is giving. The best example for this is the yucca moth, but there are probably lots of other examples I am not thinking of. Here is a link to what seems to be an interesting article, although I have only read the abstract: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1690960/
DeleteI like the idea of discussing this in class.
DeleteThis paper introduced for the very first time the groundbreaking concept of feeding behavior. I think it was a great contribution to the growing literature related to this field (2.949 citation to date).How would an organism optimize time and energy while gets food in its environment?. What is the influence of feeding behavior on the evolution of other life history traits across different clades in the tree of life?. Time minimizer made me think about north poles creatures such as polar bears with fixing breeding cycles or energy maximizers when I look hummingbirds feeding of Bursmesteiras in cloud forest in the Andes.
ReplyDeleteKat's question: Schoener makes the assumption in the general model that any organism that does not feed during a period has a reproductive output of 0. Obviously if an organism starves to death, or has no energy to mate, this is true. Are there cases where it is not?
ReplyDeleteThere's one potential example that I can think of and it's aphids. Aphids have a crazy life cycle which includes being born pregnant. This is a case where the aphid has zero feeding input at the time of birth with a potential reproductive output. I'm not sure if the aphid has to feed before giving birth, but it seems to me like an aphid's reproductive strategy would reduce (maybe eliminate) feeding input necessary for reproductive success.
"Most aphids are born pregnant and beget females without wastrel males. These parthenogenetic oocytes result from a modified meiosis that skips the reduction division, maintaining diploidy and heterozygosity. Embryos complete development within the mother’s ovary one after another, in assembly line fashion. These developing embryos contain developing embryos of the third generation within them, like Russian dolls.
DeleteOnce a year, most aphids quit this hectic lifestyle and have sex... Males are produced by another trick of asexual meiosis leading to loss of one X chromosome." http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2974440/
Thinking about Kat's question: it seems like any hibernation or diapause would constitute cessation of feeding, and would not have to compromise or eliminate future reproductive output.
DeleteAnother possible example from plants, is that some plants will, under stressful conditions, put all of their energy reserves into reproduction. They may not be photosynthesizing much, and a negative carbon balance means they effectively wouldn't be 'feeding' during that time. Not sure how much Schoener's feeding strategy theories carry over to flora though. I also think he was just stating an assumption for the models, rather than a fact of nature.
DeleteI was interested in the optimal kinds of feeders were specialist vs generalists were brought up. When qualifying an animal as being more of a generalist then another Schoener brings up omnivory as more generalist which is intuitive. He mentions the degree of omnivority in nature is dependent on food availability at a given time and also the energy demands or the animal; younger animals needing more energy to grow. He also suggests larger animals may be forced towards herbivory because of energy demands. However, larger animals and unless there is an abundance of food, larger animals would eat more diverse foods.
ReplyDeleteHere is a short article from Schoener himself in 1987, talking about synthesizing his paper and what he was thinking at the time: http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/classics1987/A1987J818600001.pdf
ReplyDeleteI don't understand the term "L" on page 413-- "the deficit in reproductive output that accrues because the animal spends time feeding rather than doing something else." If an animal can't reproduce at all when feeding is not included in its use of time, how can feeding time be considered a deficit in reproductive output?
ReplyDeleteIn this paper, I especially appreciated that under Schoener's optimization scenario, there is a point where the cost of expelling an invader from the group exceeds the cost of including the invader in the group.
I agree with Kat, I thought this was dense and long. Took a while to get through. It seems like a lot of these foundation papers are the "first" (Felisa said to be careful saying this) to attempt to quantify behavior in some kind of helpful way. I like the idea of developing predictive models because one can actually test these models so the ideas presented are less conceptual and easier to grasp.
ReplyDeleteAgreed that this paper was very dense. It was an impressively broad synthesis and discussion of a lot of neat ideas on foraging theory developed since the earlier work of Nicholson & Bailey and MacArthur & Pianka. I did find it hard to figure out what to take away from the paper (the lack of a conclusion didn't help). Perhaps this is the nature of a review/synthesis, and I'm sure it was/is extremely useful for people who study this stuff, but I'm not sure I see the original advance provided by the work. His general model seems key, though it'd be helpful to go over this in class as I'm not sure I fully grasped it. I wish he'd gone into more depth with biological examples as Harper did.
ReplyDeleteOne thing I wonder about that Schoener didn't really address relates to the nutritional quality of the food. His theory of feeding strategies is entirely energy based, but there must be situations in which populations are limited less by energy (carbohydrates) than by lack of some critical element (e.g. N, P, Ca, K etc.). This is certainly the case for plants, and I'd imagine for animals in some cases too. How do nutritional requirements influence feeding strategy?
While the paper was a bit dense and long, I think it made some intriguing points. Most particular is the idea that larger organisms should be more specialized and have larger ranges to find food. This made me think of calculations of diet breadth in archaeological sites. Humans, typically, specialize when there is an abundance of choice resources available, and then generalize as these resources become scarce. Strategies also broaden to a wider search area when it is known that choice foods are available in other areas. I am interested to learn the feeding strategies of other species and how these would relate.
ReplyDeleteNutritional requirements definitely come into play archaeologically. Cost-benefit of pursuing and processing high-ranked and medium-ranked prey greatly outweighs pursuit of lower ranked prey. When higher ranked prey are encountered, groups will always pursue them. However, everytime lower ranked prey is encountered, they will be ignored, if it is known that higher-ranked prey are available. It would have been great if Schoener would also have addressed this with other animals.